7. If we are going
to place the onus in reducing gun violence or the occurrence of mass shooting
incidents on addressing mental illness, then we have to have a clear conception
of WHAT MENTAL ILLNESS IS and FROM WHOM WE ARE TRYING TO IMPEDE ACCESS TO FIREARMS.
The Roberts Court opens
the door with Heller by making the critical exception, in its reading of
an individual right to bear arms in the Second Amendment, regarding regulation
of firearms possession by the mentally ill and felons. Leaving felons aside for the purposes of my
argument as a relatively sensible exception (provided we believe that prison
systems nominally established with the purpose of rehabilitating criminals will
always, invariably, fail to transform, presumably, ingrained predispositions
toward criminality in the minds of convicts), the idea that legislatures should
be empowered to inhibit access to firearms by the mentally ill deserves some
scrutiny. Specifically, who exactly are
we going to be preventing from using guns? Is our definition of mental illness too broad or too
narrow to achieve the goals that we have in mind in prohibiting
access? Just what is it that defines
mental illness?
There seem to be some obvious symptomatic candidates here
arising from the genetic inheritance of bio-chemically-induced cognitive
abnormalities. Such instances might
include schizophrenics and other individuals relying on the continuous and
permanent use of anti-psychotic medications.
It probably makes good sense to prevent the legal purchase or possession
of firearms by such individuals if only because their possession of the
capacity to inflict lethal physical violence might generate highly
unpredictable patterns of use, endangering others and themselves. Nationally, approximately 2.2 million
individuals suffer from clinically diagnosed schizophrenia (see “Schizophrenia
Facts and Statistics” at: http://www.schizophrenia.com/szfacts.htm).
Notwithstanding apparent difficulties in diagnosing such conditions,
relative to other anxiety-driven mental/emotional conditions, this scale of
illness appears susceptible to effective regulatory control, at least with
respect to the purchase of firearms. On
the other hand, there are clearly diagnostic issues in the identification of a
disease like schizophrenia, relative to chronic depression, bipolar disorder,
and other conditions. Moreover, I
simply do not understand the capabilities and social limitations of
schizophrenics and individuals with similar mental/emotional conditions in such
a way that I could definitively argue that they merit legal restriction from
firearm use. Sadly, I am not a
psychiatrist – just a deli clerk and a Marxist political-economist/urbanist. With my own limitations in mind, permit me
to contribute some intellectual speculation on mental illness based on what I
think I do understand about bio-chemical signatures/correlations, behavioral
symptoms, and socio-environmental inducements that might complicate the
discussion if we are contemplating what individuals should be allowed access to
firearms and who should not be.
Bio-chemically speaking, one thing that interests me a
lot is cortisol. Cortisol is a hormone
produced by the adrenal gland, regulating levels of glucose within the
bloodstream. When a “normal” individual
begins to experience stress, the adrenal gland increases cortisol
production. The increase both elevates
the level of glucose within the bloodstream and reduces the immuno-suppressive
capacity of the body, enabling the body to devote an increased quantity of
energy to relieve the socio-environmental sources of the stress. For this reason, cortisol is frequently
referred to as the “fight or flight hormone.”
Pleading some degree of ignorance on the chemistry of cortisol,
its activation pathways, and its particular effects on organic systems, I am
interested in some of the research done within various academic and clinical
settings linking cortisol production to stress, particular psychological conditions
like post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and connections between cortisol
levels and levels of other bodily hormones, like testosterone, as bio-chemical
precursors to socially aggressive behaviors.
Summarizing the very small subset of studies on
cortisol (mostly literature reviews) that I have examined, certain patterns are
evident. First, baseline (basal)
cortisol levels in the blood stream and stress-reactive cortisol secretions appear
to correlate positively to pro-social, empathetic behavior (see Shirtcliff et
al. (2009)). Conversely, anti-social,
callous behavior patterns appear to correlate with lower basal levels of
cortisol and lower reactive secretions.
Relating such behavioral patterns to environmental contexts, cortisol
secretions, arising from reactivity to stressors, appear to respond both
to stressors impacting the individual and to stressors impacting others in
close association to the individual (i.e. friends, loved ones – Shirtcliff
et al. (2009) cite Sethre-Hofstad et al. (2002), who note enhanced cortisol
responses by mothers watching their young children undergo stressful
situations). Additionally, basal
cortisol levels and reactive secretions appear to respond over time to
extreme/traumatic stress and to chronically stressful conditions in ways that
tend to readjust adrenal cortisol production (see Miller et al. (2007)). As a result, individuals experiencing
post-traumatic stress disorder, within at least certain studies, display lower
levels of basal cortisol and a higher reactive threshold for reactive cortisol
secretions. Finally, some relationship
appears to exist between basal cortisol levels and testosterone in the
prevalence of aggressive behaviors (see Popma et al. (2007)). In particular, higher levels of basal
cortisol appear to moderate aggressive behaviors linked to high testosterone
production.
Acknowledging that anyone interpreting my conclusions should
exercise a high degree of caution given my lack of any formal
education in psychiatry or bio-chemistry, the evidence that I have read
seems to suggest that individuals who have experienced either traumatic or
chronic stress, resulting in lower basal cortisol levels and higher reactive
thresholds for cortisol secretion, with simultaneous high levels of
testosterone production, will be both more prone to aggressive and anti-social
behavior and significantly less apt to experience empathy (and, thus, remorse
for their actions). Supplementing these
insights, normal production of testosterone, measured in nanograms per
deciliter in the bloodstream, by post-pubescent males exceeds post-pubescent
female production, on average, by at least a factor of ten, and male
testosterone levels tend to reach a maximum between ages 16 and 30. Thus, males, between the ages of 16 and 30,
emerging from one or multiple episodes of chronic or traumatic stress, will be
likely to combine high levels of testosterone production with lower levels of
basal cortisol and an elevated threshold for reactive cortisol secretion. In fact, evaluating homicide statistics in
the U.S. from 1980 to 2008, males constitute 89.5 percent of all offenders and
individuals between the ages of 14 and 34 constitute 78.1 percent of all
offenders (see Cooper and Smith (2011), at: http://www.bjs.gov/content/pub/pdf/htus8008.pdf).
Implicitly, without actually breaking out Bureau of Justice Statistics
(BJS) homicide victimization and offender series and performing regression
analysis, there would seem to be some statistically significant correlation
between gender-specific average patterns in the level of testosterone
concentration in the bloodstream and the gender and age-specific prevalence of
lethal violence, but the role of cortisol does not emerge here. If cortisol mediates the socio-behavioral
effects of high testosterone production, then the presence of incidents or
circumstances that result in the diminution of basal cortisol and the elevation
of thresholds for cortisol secretion, specifically in males between the ages
of 16 and 30, should send out a red flag for potential danger in granting
access to firearms! So, what sorts of
stressful incidents or circumstances might impact cortisol production?
This is the point where I want to transition from the
bio-chemical realm of cortisol and testosterone to territory that I feel at
least a little bit more comfortable dealing with as a social scientist: social
contexts that might place individuals in a state of emotional duress or inflict
long-term/chronic personal anxiety. For
some reason, the mainstream media and various political constituencies,
particularly right-wing conservative ones supportive of gun rights and
otherwise looking for a good scapegoat, have centered their attention on video
games. The logic here is that
playing one of the current generation of violent, bloody video games will
generate a traumatizing effect on young minds, stressing and subsequently
realigning cortisol production in ways that will diminish the mediating
capacity of cortisol in developing minds.
As reasonable as this argument seems, the actual evidence for effects
from video games is mixed. An Iranian
research team has recently confronted the question of effects from violent
video games on cortisol production in males head-on and concluded that violent
games do, in fact, elevate levels of cortisol for an extended period of
time, relative to cortisol levels from playing non-violent video games (see
Hossini et al. (2011)). On the other
side, a Swedish team performed a research experiment under similar conditions,
with a slightly younger (12-15 years rather than 17-19 in the Iranian sampling)
and smaller (21 rather than 50) male population sampling for a longer period (2
hours rather than 30 minutes) with comparably designed sampling frequencies for
retrieval of salivary cortisol samples and found no significant statistical difference between the
effects of violent and non-violent video games on cortisol production (see
Ivarsson (2009)). Given the mixed
nature of evidence on the effects of violent video games on cortisol, it seems
more likely that the attention that has been given to video games as a “smoking
gun” in the social inducement of violent male behavior, in general, and gun
violence, in particular, has been a “red herring” – in political rhetoric, it
is always more satisfying to find a logically convincing scapegoat for a
significant public policy problem than to admit that the problem is much more
complex and nuanced to ever be adequately addressed with available statutory
tools.
Socially speaking, my big concern is not with the
effects of video games on young minds.
That is not to say that exposing young men to violent images from video games or movies does
not produce potential traumatic stress.
Rather, I am convinced that there are many other social
processes, present intermittently but with imposing effects that, in
combinations, will expose individuals at various ages to overwhelming levels of
stress. Lets start with a range of
processes with which I am much more familiar – those involving the functioning
of regional macroeconomies in the present era of economic globalization. In this respect, I am talking about
variability in rates of economic growth, employment, measurements of income
inequality (e.g. Gini coefficients), reflecting impacts on individual workers of the increasing
openness of regional economies with the rest of the global economy. How does the turbulence of economic growth,
employment, and income in American metropolitan regional economies impose personal
stresses on males between the ages of 16 and 30, and how are these stresses
negotiated? Evidence does exist for
higher basal cortisol levels among long-term unemployed individuals relative to
individuals in stable employment (see Detteborn (2010)), but, to my knowledge,
neuroendocrinology researchers have never broached this question in reference
to target groups of unemployed young men.
What about chronically unemployed young men in urban communities
characterized by significantly below average income levels and higher than
average community unemployment rates?
What about young, male college graduates, continuously unemployed or
under-employed (relative to average compensation rates for college graduates in
their field) for at least one year following graduation, with over, say, $25,000 in educational
debt? The burden of fundamental
uncertainty may hover over the heads of hedge fund managers, but it must also,
invariably, rest on the minds and provoke unmanageable stress for young men in
chronically socially disadvantaged communities and, even, for young men in
labor markets that have not offered immediate returns on the promise of higher
education.
In evaluating the possibilities for social inducement of
chronic and traumatic stress on young men, I would be amiss if I did not
venture into the realm of love and sexuality.
How does the variability of sexual relations in the lives of young men
impact cortisol production? In
particular, how do variations in the frequency and quality of sexual relations
(e.g. dating, monogamous long-term sexual relationships with and without
fathering, polygamous relations) constitute a source of chronic or traumatic
stress for young men? If, in this
regard, testosterone production correlates positively to both the
intensity of sexual desire and propensities toward aggressive behavior in
males, then sexuality, sexual behavior, and multifarious sexuality-derived
sources of stress must constitute a significant part of the equation in
considering the regulation of cortisol and its mediating role relative to aggression. Returning briefly to BJS statistics on
homicide from 1980 to 2008, intimates (i.e. spouses, ex-spouses, or
boyfriends/girlfriends) are involved in 41.5 percent of homicides in which the
victim is female, compared to 16.7 percent of female homicides accountable to
other family members, 29.9 percent accountable to other acquaintances, and 11.9
percent to strangers. At issue here are
the bio-chemical effects of jealous, possessive behavior by males and their manifestations
in domestic violence against partners (presumably both heterosexual and
homosexual).
It is not my intention in this section to lay the
psychological onus of gun violence entirely on the bio-chemical relationship
between cortisol and testosterone.
Rather, my point is that, if we want to emphasize the role of mental
illness in incidents like Newtown, Aurora, Virginia Tech, or Columbine and
utilize mental illness as a parameter in determining who should and should not
have access to firearms, then we need to ask, more broadly, what mental illness
is and how social conditions might contribute to the manifestation of mental
illnesses through the functioning of physiologically known bio-chemical
mechanisms in order to determine who might be at risk for committing violent
behavior and, thus, whose access to firearms should be restricted. If, in this regard, we are not going to just
scapegoat schizophrenics or individuals taking anti-psychotic
medications (and, further, ask what sort of individuals might benefit from psychiatric treatment but have never actually sought treatment), then we need to seriously look at a much broader range of
socio-environmental risk factors (e.g. economic, sexual, cultural, familial)
contributing to unmanageable stress, specifically in young males. Such evaluations, if seriously pursued,
would be likely to vastly expand the range of individuals under suspicion for the potential to utilize firearms in the
performance of both impulsive and premeditated violent criminal
acts.
Having made the argument here that our
psychiatric/pharmacological definition of mental illness is far too
narrow to impact the prevalence of gun violence by restricting access by
mentally ill individuals, I am equally confident that, in constitutionally
evaluating restrictions on access to firearms by the mentally ill, the Roberts
Court and, by recourse to precedent, the federal judiciary, in general, will
apply the much more strict psychiatric definition of mental illness that I am
rejecting here as too narrow.
As with the scapegoating of video game designers, it is much easier and more satisfying for both conservative politicians and jurists to accept a clinically clear boundary between sane, rational people and the mentally ill, who have to take medications and undergo therapy. If regulation of firearms access under psychiatric/pharmacological standards is apt to accomplish anything, it may, thus, be expected to reinforce social stigmas associated with emotional disorders and dissuade individuals who might otherwise benefit from anti-depressants or therapy from seeking help. Therefore, the idea of saving the lives of potential victims in mass
shooting incidents, to say nothing of saving the lives of women involved in
domestic abuse and murdered by their partners with a gun, by preventing mentally
ill individuals from accessing guns seems entirely ludicrous within the present political and constitutional context.
No comments:
Post a Comment